Perceptual Development

Just a century ago it was widely believed that the world perceived by newborn infants was, in the words of WILLIAM JAMES, a "blooming, buzzing confusion." In the decades since then, developmental research has demonstrated dramatically that James's view was erroneous. The shift in view was prompted by research from various domains. In the 1930s, Piaget's detailed descriptions of his infant children and Gesell's charting of infants' motor milestones created a climate of interest in infants as research subjects and in developmental questions. The work of ethologists studying the behavior of animals in their natural habitats (COMPARATIVE PSYCHOLOGY) paved the way for careful observations of spontaneous activity in even the youngest animals. Observation of spontaneous activity ran counter to theories of stimulus-response (S-R) chaining and the radical BEHAVIORISM fashionable in the 1930s; at the same time, it inspired the design of new methods for studying infants, including methods for asking what infants perceive.

By the 1960s, methods for studying infant perception had multiplied as psychologists exploited infants' natural exploratory behaviors, especially looking. Preferential looking at one of two displays, habituation to one display followed by a new display, and a paired comparison test of old and new displays were highly effective methods for studying visual discrimination of simple contrasting properties and even more complex patterns. Spontaneous exploratory behavior was also the basis for research methods, particularly operant conditioning of responses such as sucking, head turning, or moving a limb. Methods which provide infants with opportunities to control their environment (e.g., operant conditioning, infant-controlled habituation) were shown to be more effective than methods without consequences for changing behavior (Horowitz et al. 1972). Psychologists found that they could also investigate what is perceived utilizing natural actions in controlled experimental situations, such as reaching for objects varying in bulk or attainability, and locomotion across surfaces varying in rigidity, pitfalls, obstacles, and slope. Methods borrowed from physiological research, including heart rate and electrophysiological responses, have been used effectively in studying sensitivity to change in stimulus dimensions. These measures, along with psychophysical procedures, have revealed impressive discriminatory abilities in very young infants. (See VISION AND LEARNING; AUDITION; TASTE; and SMELL.) Researchers are now discovering the precursors of some of these competencies during the fetal period of prenatal development.

Major research topics include development of perception of events, the persistent properties of objects, and the larger layout of surfaces. Five key points that emerge are the following:

(1) The perception of events is prospective or forward-looking. One example is infants' differential response to approaching obstacles and apertures, the so-called looming studies (Yonas, Petterson, and Lockman 1979). Infants respond with defensive blinking and head retraction to approaching objects, but not to approaching apertures or to withdrawing objects. Studies of neonates reaching out to catch moving objects provide another compelling demonstration of anticipatory perception as skilled reaching develops (Hofsten 1983). Infants also anticipate occurrence of environmental happenings, for example, by looking toward the locus of a predictable event (Haith 1993).

(2) Motion is important for revealing the persistent properties of events, objects, and layout of the world. A striking example is the perception of biological motion when visual information is minimized. When spots of light are placed on key joints (e.g., elbows, ankles, hips), and all other illumination is eliminated, observers immediately perceive a person engaging in a uniquely specified activity, such as walking, dancing, or lifting a heavy box, but only when the actor is moving. Infants differentiate these biological motion displays from inverted displays and from spots of light moving in a random fashion (Bertenthal 1993). Motion makes possible pickup of information about social (communicative) events, such as smiling and talking, at an early age. The role of motion is also critical in visual detection of constant properties of objects, such as unity, size, shape (SHAPE PERCEPTION), and substance. At four months of age, infants perceive the unity of an object despite partial occlusion by another object, provided that the occluded object is in motion (Kellman and Spelke 1983). Constant size of an object is given in changes in distance relative to self and background, and neonates appear to detect size as invariant (Slater, Mattock, and Brown 1990). Shape constancy, invariant over changes in orientation of an object, is perceived by five months (Gibson et al. 1979). Rigidity or elasticity of substance is differentiated via mouthing in neonates (Rochat 1983), and visually by five months (Gibson and Walker 1984). Methods of visual habituation and observation of grasping skills converge on evidence for perceiving these properties. Such convergence is not surprising, since exploration of object properties is naturally multimodal. Surface properties of objects, such as color (see COLOR VISION), are not necessarily dependent on motion, but texture of an object's surface is accessed by haptic as well as visual information and is differentiated early in the first year (SURFACE PERCEPTION; see also HAPTIC PERCEPTION).

(3) Perception is multimodally unified (MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION). From the earliest moments of life, infants orient to sounds, particularly human voices, and they engage in active visual exploration of faces and sounding objects (Gibson 1988). In fact, infants as young as five months can match the sounds and visible motions of faces and voices in a bimodal matching task (Kuhl and Meltzoff 1982; Walker 1982). Infants can also match the sounds and visible motions of object events (Spelke 1976), evidently perceiving a unified event. At one month, infants appear to detect and unify haptic and visual information for object substance (Gibson and Walker 1984).

(4) Properties of the larger layout are made available multimodally as motor skills and new action patterns develop. Experience and practice play an important role in this development. How far away things are must be perceived in units of body scale by infants. Observation of the hands, in relation to surrounding objects, occurs spontaneously within the first month (van der Meer, van der Weel, and Lee 1995). When reaching for objects emerges as a skill, judging not only the distance of an object but its size improves rapidly. Information for the major properties of the layout is best accessed when babies begin locomotion. While recognition of obstacles, approaching objects, and surface properties is not unprepared, experience in traversing the ground surface brings new lessons. Crawling infants tend to avoid a steep drop in the surface of support (Gibson and Walk 1960). The affordance of falling is perceived early in locomotor history, but becomes more dependable with experience in locomotion. Properties of the surface of support that afford locomotion (its rigidity, smoothness, slope, etc.) are detected by experienced crawling infants. They learn to cope effectively with steep slopes by avoiding them or adopting safe methods of travel, but the same infants as novice upright walkers attempt dangerous slopes and must learn new strategies (Adolph 1997). Bipedal locomotion requires extensive adjustments of perceptual and locomotor skills, as infants learn a new balancing act, using multimodal information from ankles, joints, and visual cues provided by flow patterns created by their own movements. Novice walkers fall down in a "moving room," despite a firm and stable unmoving ground surface (Lee and Aronson 1974). Flow patterns created by the room's motion give false information that they are falling forward or backward. Perceiving the world entails coperception of the self; in this case, via visual information from perspective changes in the room's walls in relation to vestibular information about one's own upright posture.

(5) Infants perceive the SELF as a unit distinct from the world (see SELF-KNOWLEDGE). By four to five months, infants watch their own legs moving currently on a television screen, contrasted with views of similarly clad legs of another infant or their own at an earlier moment (Bahrick and Watson 1985). They reliably prefer to gaze at the novel display rather than their own ongoing movements. However, introduction of a target that can be kicked changes the preference to monitoring the ongoing self kicking at the target (Morgan and Rochat 1995). An opportunity for making contact with an object provides motivation for controlling the encounter. Considerable other research in a contingent reinforcement situation (e.g., kicking to rotate a mobile) confirms infants' perception of a self in control. Disruption of control results in frustration and emotional disturbance (Lewis, Sullivan, and Brooks-Gunn 1985).

Early reaction to the explosion of knowledge about the perceptual abilities of young infants was a burst of astonished admiration ("Aren't babies wonderful?"), and little concern was given to how development progresses, although previously popular Piagetian views were questioned. Three current views vary in their assumptions about processes involved in perceptual development. Two are construction theories: (1) The information processing view assumes that bare sensory input is subject to cognitive processing that constructs meaningful perception. (2) The nativist view assumes that rules about order governing events in the world are inherently given and used to interpret observed events. (3) The third view combines an ecological approach to perception and a systems view. Infants actively seek information that comes to specify identities, places, and affordances in the world. Processes that influence development are the progressive growth and use of action systems, and learning through experience. Perceptual learning is viewed as a selective process, beginning with exploratory activity, leading to observation of consequences, and to selection based on two criteria, an affordance fit and reduction of uncertainty, exemplified by detection of order and unity in what is perceived.

We know much less about perceptual development after the first two years. After infancy, perceptual development takes place mainly in complex tasks such as athletic skills, tool use, way-finding, steering vehicles, using language, and READING -- all tasks in which experience and learning become more and more specialized (cf. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT). Theoretical applications to specialized tasks involving perceptual learning can be profitable (Abernathy 1993).

See also

-- Eleanor J. Gibson, Marion Eppler, and Karen Adolph

References

Abernathy, B. (1993). Searching for the minimal information for skilled perception and action. Psychological Research 55:131-138.

Adolph, K. E. (1997). Learning in the Development of Infant Locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Serial No. 251, vol. 62, no. 3.

Bahrick, L. E., and J. S. Watson. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology 21:963-973.

Bertenthal, B. (1993). Infants' perception of biological motions. In C. Granrud, Ed., Visual Perception and Cognition in Infancy. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 175-214.

Gibson, E. J. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting and the acquiring of knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology 39:1-41.

Gibson, E. J., and R. D. Walk. (1960). The "visual cliff." Scientific American 202:64-71.

Gibson, E. J., and A. S. Walker. (1984). Development of knowledge of visual-tactual affordances of substance. Child Development 55:453-460.

Gibson, E. J., C. J. Owsley, A. S. Walker, and J. S. Megaw-Nyce. (1979). Development of the perception of invariants: Substance and shape. Perception 5:609-619.

Haith, M. M. (1993). Future-oriented processes in infancy: The case of visual expectations. In C. Granrud, Ed., Visual Perception and Cognition in Infancy. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 235-264.

Hofsten, C. von (1983). Catching skills in infancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2:75-85.

Horowitz, F., L. Paden, W. Bhana, and P. Self. (1972). An "infant control" procedure for studying infant visual fixations. Developmental Psychology 7: 90.

Kellman, P. J., and E. Spelke. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology 15:483-524.

Kuhl, P. K., and A. N. Meltzoff. (1982). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science 218:1138-1141.

Lee, D. N., and E. Aronson. (1974). Visual proprioceptive control of standing in human infants. Perception and Psychophysics 15:529-532.

Lewis, M., M. W. Sullivan, and J. Brooks-Gunn. (1985). Emotional behavior during the teaming of a contingency in early infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 3:307-316.

Morgan, R., and P. Rochat. (1995). The perception of self-produced leg movements in self- vs. object-oriented contexts by 3- to 5-month-old infants. In B. C. Bardy, R. J. Bootsma, and Y. Guiard, Eds., Studies in Perception and Action 3. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rochat, P. (1983). Oral touch in young infants: Response to variations of nipple characteristics in the first month of life. International Journal of Behavioral Development 6:123-133.

Slater, A., A. Mattock, and E. Brown. (1990). Size constancy at birth: Newborn infants' responses to retinal and real size. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 49:314-322.

Spelke, E. S. (1976). Infants' intermodal perception of events. Cognitive Psychology 8:533-560.

van der Meer, A. L. H., F. R. van der Weel, and D. N. Lee. (1995). The functional significance of arm movements in neonates. Science 267:693-695.

Walker, A. S. (1982). Intermodal perception of expressive behavior by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 23:514-535.

Yonas, A., L. Petterson, and J. Lockman. (1979). Young infants' sensitivity to optical information for collision. Canadian Journal of Psychology 33:1285-1290.

Further Readings

Bertenthal, B., and R. K. Clifton. (1997). Perception and action. In D. Kuhn and R. Siegler, Eds., Handbook of Child Psychology, vol. 2: Cognition, Perception and Language. New York: Wiley, pp. 51-102.

Bertenthal, B. (1996). Origins and early development of perception, action, and representation. Annual Reviews of Psychology 47:431-459.

Fantz, R. L. (1961). The origins of form perception. Scientific American 204:66-72.

Gesell, A. (1928). Infancy and Human Growth. New York: Macmillan.

Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Gibson, E. J., and E. S. Spelke. (1983). The development of perception. In P. H. Mussen, Ed., Handbook of Child Psychology, 4th ed., vol. 3: Cognitive Development. New York: Wiley, pp. 1-76.

Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics 14:201-211.

Piaget, J. (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York: International Universities Press.

Teller, D. Y. (1979). The forced-choice preferential looking procedure: A psychophysical technique for use with human infants. Infant Behavior and Development 2:135-153 .