Memory, Animal Studies

Information about which structures and connections in the brain are important for MEMORY has come from studies of amnesiac patients and from systematic experimental work with animals. Work in animals includes studies which assess the effects of selective brain lesions on memory, as well as studies using neurophysiological recording and stimulating techniques to investigate neural activity within particular brain regions (for discussions of the latter two approaches, see OBJECT RECOGNITION, ANIMAL STUDIES; FACE RECOGNITION; SINGLE-NEURON RECORDING). An important development that has occurred in the area of memory during the past two decades was the establishment of an animal model of human amnesia in the monkey (Mahut and Moss 1984; Mishkin 1982; Squire and Zola-Morgan 1983). In the 1950s, Scoville and Milner (1957) described the severe amnesia that followed bilateral surgical removal of the medial temporal lobe (patient H.M.). This important case demonstrated that memory is a distinct cerebral function, dissociable from other perceptual and cognitive abilities.

In monkeys, surgical lesions of the medial temporal lobe, which were intended to approximate the damage sustained by patient H.M., reproduced many features of human memory impairment. In particular, both monkeys and humans were impaired on tasks of declarative memory, but fully intact at skills and habit learning and other tasks of nondeclarative memory. This achievement set the stage for additional work in monkeys and for work in rodents that has identified structures in the medial temporal lobe that are important for declarative memory. These structures include the hippocampal region (i.e., the cell fields of the HIPPOCAMPUS, the dentate gyrus, and the subiculum), and adjacent cortical areas that are anatomically related to the hippocampal region, namely, the entorhinal, perirhinal, and parahippocampal cortices (Zola-Morgan and Squire 1993).

The midline diencephalon is another brain area important for memory, although less is known about which specific structures in this region contribute to memory function. Findings from work in animals, including the development of an animal model of alcoholic Korsakoff's syndrome in the rat (Mair et al., 1992), have been consistent with the anatomical findings from human amnesia in showing the importance of damage within the medial THALAMUS, especially damage in the internal medullary lamina, for producing memory loss. Lesions in the internal medullary lamina would be expected to disconnect or damage several thalamic nuclei, including intralaminar nuclei, the mediodorsal nucleus, and the anterior nucleus (Aggleton and Mishkin 1983; Mair et al. 1991; Zola-Morgan and Squire 1985). However, the separate contributions to memory of the mediodorsal nucleus, the anterior nucleus, and the intralaminar nuclei remain to be explored systematically with well-circumscribed lesions in animals.

A major criterion for demonstrating that an animal has a memory deficit is to show that performance is impaired at long-delay intervals, but is intact at short-delay intervals, that is, no impairment in perception, attention, or general intellectual function. A successful strategy for demonstrating intact short-term memory and impaired long-term memory has involved training normal monkeys and monkeys with medial temporal lobe lesions on the delayed nonmatching -to-sample task, a recognition memory task sensitive to amnesia in humans. In this task, the monkey first sees an object, and then after a prescribed delay the animal is given a choice between the previously seen object and a novel one. The key feature of this experimental approach is the use of very short delay intervals (e.g., 0.5 sec). The absence of an impairment at a delay of 0.5 sec would indicate that the medial temporal lobe lesions do not affect short-term memory. Using this strategy, Alvarez-Royo, Zola-Morgan, and Squire (1992) and Overman, Ormsby, and Mishkin (1990) showed that medial temporal lobe lesions impair memory at long delays, but not at very short delays. Studies in rats using delayed nonmatching-to-sample as well as a variety of other memory tasks have also demonstrated that long-term memory is impaired while short-term memory is spared following lesions that involve the hippocampal region (Kesner and Novak 1982; for recent reviews of work in rats, see Further Readings). These findings underscore the idea that medial temporal lobe lesions reproduce a key feature of human amnesia, that is, the distinction between intact short-term memory and impaired long-term memory.

It was originally supposed that damage to the AMYGDALA directly contributed to the memory impairment associated with large medial temporal lobe lesions (Murray and Mishkin 1984). Subsequent work showed that monkeys with virtually complete lesions of the amygdala performed as well as normal monkeys on four different memory tasks, including delayed nonmatching-to-sample task (Zola- Morgan et al. 1989). Other experiments with rats and monkeys suggest that the amygdala is important for other kinds of memory, including the development of conditioned fear and other forms of affective memory (see EMOTION AND THE ANIMAL BRAIN). These and other findings (Murray 1992) focused attention away from the amygdala toward the cortical structures of the medial temporal lobe, that is, the perirhinal, entorhinal, and parahippocampal cortices, in addition to the hippocampal region itself.

Direct evidence for the importance of the cortical regions has come from studies in which circumscribed damage has been done to the perirhinal, entorhinal, or parahippocampal cortices, either separately or in combination (Moss, Mahut, and Zola-Morgan 1981; Zola-Morgan et al. 1989; Gaffan and Murray 1992; Meunier et al. 1993; Suzuki et al. 1993; Leonard et al. 1995). For example, monkeys with combined lesions of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices exhibited severe, multimodal, and long-lasting memory impairment (Zola-Morgan et al. 1989; Suzuki et al. 1993). More limited lesions of the cortical regions also produce memory impairment. For example, several studies found that monkeys with bilateral lesions limited to the perirhinal cortex exhibit long-lasting memory impairment (Meunier et al. 1993; Ramus, Zola-Morgan, and Squire 1994). Additionally, a large number of individual studies in monkeys and in rats with varying extents of damage to the medial temporal lobe, together with work in humans, has led to the idea that the severity of memory impairment increases as more components of the medial temporal lobe memory system are damaged.

A long-standing and controversial issue in work on memory has been whether the hippocampal region is disproportionately involved in spatial memory, or whether spatial memory is simply a good example of a broader category of memory that requires the hippocampal region. One view of the matter comes from earlier work with monkeys (Parkinson, Murray, and Mishkin 1988). Monkeys with lesions that involved the hippocampal formation (hippocampus plus underlying posterior entorhinal cortex and parahippocampal cortex) were severely impaired in acquiring an object-place association task, whereas lesions that involved the amygdala plus underlying anterior entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex were only mildly impaired. The authors suggested that the hippocampus has an especially important role in spatial memory, an idea developed originally by O'Keefe and Nadel (1978), based mostly on rat work. It was unclear from this monkey study, however, whether the observed spatial deficit was due to hippocampal damage, the adjacent cortical damage, or both. Additional work from both humans and animals suggests another view. In one formal study (Cave and Squire 1991), spatial memory was found to be proportionately impaired in amnesiac patients relative to object recognition memory and object recall memory. The same (nonspatial) view of hippocampal function has also been proposed for the rat, based, for example, on demonstrated deficits in odor memory tasks after ibotenate hippo campal lesions (Bunsey and Eichenbaum 1996). The role of the hippocampus in spatial memory remains unclear. Recent commentaries on the issue of the hippocampus and spatial memory can be found under Further Reading.

Uncertainty about the function of the hippocampus has been due, in part, to the inability until recently to make circumscribed lesions limited to the hippocampal region in experimental animals. Studies in which selective lesions of the hippocampal region could be accomplished became possible only with the development of (a) a technique for producing restricted ibotenate lesions of the hippocampus in the rat and (b) a technique that uses MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING to guide the placement of radiofrequency or ibotenic acid stereotaxic lesions of the hippocampal region in the monkey. Monkeys with bilateral, radiofrequency lesions of the hippocampal region, which spared almost entirely the perirhinal, entorhinal, and parahippocampal cortices, exhibited impaired performance at long delays (ten minutes and forty minutes) on the delayed nonmatching-to-sample task (Alvarez, Zola-Morgan, and Squire 1995).

Ibotenic acid lesions cause cell death but, unlike radio-frequency lesions, spare afferent and efferent white matter fibers within the region of the lesion. If it should turn out, after systematic study, that ibotenic acid lesions of the hippocampal region do not impair performance on the delayed nonmatching task, the interpretation of such studies should not be overstated. The results concern recognition memory, not memory in general, and only the kind of recognition memory measured by the nonmatching-to-sample task itself. The delayed nonmatching task has been extraordinarily useful for evaluating the effects on visual recognition memory of damage to the medial temporal lobe memory system and for measuring the severity of recognition memory impairment. However, in the case of human memory, recognition memory tests are known to be rather easy and not as sensitive to memory impairment as other tests, for instance, tests of recall or cued recall. The issue of task sensitivity is crucially important. Other kinds of recognition memory tasks, for example, the paired comparisons task (a task of spontaneous novelty preference; Bachevalier, Brickson, and Hagger 1993) and tasks that are thought to be more sensitive than tasks of simple recognition memory, for example the transverse patterning, the transitive inference, and naturalistic association tasks, have recently been developed to assess memory in animals.

An important question with respect to the components of the medial temporal lobe memory system is whether these structures all share similar functions as part of a common memory system, or do they have distinct and dissociable functions? In this regard, one must consider the neuroanatomy  of the medial temporal lobe system and its pattern of connectivity with association cortex. An extensive anatomical investigation by Suzuki and Amaral (1994) showed that different areas of neocortex gain access to the medial temporal lobe memory system at different points. Visual  information arrives preferentially to perirhinal cortex. Approximately 65 percent of the input reaching the perirhinal cortex is unimodal visual information, mostly from TE and TEO. By contrast, about 40 percent of the input reaching parahippocampal cortex is visual, mostly from area V4. Cortical areas that are believed to be important for processing spatial information project preferentially to parahippo- campal cortex. Approximately 8 percent of the input to parahippocampal cortex originates in the parietal cortex, whereas virtually none of the input to perirhinal cortex originates in the parietal cortex. These anatomical considerations lead to the expectation that perirhinal cortical lesions might impair visual memory more than spatial memory and that the reverse might be true for parahippocampal cortex. Furthermore, because both the perirhinal and the parahippocampal cortices project to the hippocampus, one might expect that hippocampal damage will similarly impair visual memory and spatial memory. The establishment of new, more sensitive behavioral tests and the development of new techniques for producing selective brain lesions have now made it possible to address these possibilities and to systematically clarify the separate contributions to memory of structures in the medial temporal lobe and the diencephalon.

See also

Additional links

-- Stuart Zola

References

Aggleton, J. P., and M. Mishkin. (1983). Memory impairments following restricted medial thalamic lesions. Exp. Brain Res. 52:199-209.

Alvarez-Royo, P., S. Zola-Morgan, and L. R. Squire. (1992). Impairment of long-term memory and sparing of short-term memory in monkeys with medial temporal lobe lesions: A response to Ringo. Behav. Brain Res. 52:1-5.

Alvarez, P., S. Zola-Morgan, and L. R. Squire. (1995). Damage limited to the hippocampal region produces long-lasting memory impairment. J. Neurosci. 15:3796-3807.

Bachevalier, J., M. Brickson, and C. Hagger. (1993). Limbic-dependent recognition memory in monkeys develops early in infancy. Neuroreport 4:77-80.

Bunsey, M., and H. Eichenbaum. (1996). Conservation of hippo-campal memory function in rats and humans. Nature 379:255-257.

Cave, C. B., and L. R. Squire. (1991). Equivalent impairment of spatial and nonspatial memory following damage to the human hippocampus. Hippocampus 1:329-340.

Gaffan, D., and E. A. Murray. (1992). Monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) with rhinal cortex ablations succeed in object discrimination learning despite 24-hr intertrial intervals and fail at matching to sample despite double sample presentations. Behav. Neurosci. 106:30-38.

Kesner, R. P., and J. M. Novak. (1982). Serial position curve in rats: Role of the dorsal hippocampus. Science 218:173-175.

Leonard, B. W., D. G. Amaral, L. R. Squire, and S. Zola-Morgan. (1995). Transient memory impairment in monkeys with bilateral lesions of the entorhinal cortex. J. Neurosci. 15:5637-5659.

Mahut, H., and M. Moss. (1984). Consolidation of memory: The hippocampus revisited. In L. R. Squire and N. Butters, Eds., Neuropsychology of Memory, vol 1. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 297-315.

Mair, R. G., R. L. Knoth, S. A. Rabehenuk, and P. J. Lanlais. (1991). Impairment of olfactory, auditory, and spatial serial reversal learning in rats recovered from pyrithiamine induced thiamine deficiency. Behav. Neurosci. 105:360-374.

Mair, R. G., J. K. Robinson, S. M. Koger, G. D. Fox, and Y. P. Zhang. (1992). Delayed non-matching to sample is impaired by extensive, but not by limited lesions of thalamus in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 106:646-656.

Meunier, M., J. Bachevalier, M. Mishkin, and E. A. Murray. (1993). Effects on visual recognition of combined and separate ablations of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortex in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 13:5418-5432.

Mishkin, M. (1982). A memory system in the monkey. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 98:85-95.

Moss, M., H. Mahut, and S. Zola-Morgan. (1981). Concurrent discrimination learning of monkeys after hippocampal, entorhinal, or fornix lesions. J. Neurosci. 1:227-240.

Murray, E. A. (1992). Medial temporal lobe structures contributing to recognition memory: The amygdaloid complex versus the rhinal complex. In J. P. Aggleton, Ed., The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss, pp. 453-470.

Murray, E. A., and M. Mishkin. (1984). Severe tactual as well as visual memory deficits following combined removal of the amygdala and hippocampus in monkeys. J. Neurosci. 4:2565-2580.

O'Keefe, J., and L. Nadel. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Overman, W. H., G. Ormsby, and M. Mishkin. (1990). Picture recognition vs. picture discrimination learning in monkeys with medial temporal removals. Exp. Brain Res. 79:18-24.

Parkinson, J. K., E. A. Murray, and M. Mishkin. (1988). A selective mnemonic role for the hippocampus in monkeys: Memory for the location of objects. J. Neurosci. 8:4159-4167.

Ramus, S. J., S. Zola-Morgan, and L. R. Squire. (1994). Effects of lesions of perirhinal cortex or parahippocampal cortex on memory in monkeys. Soc. Neurosci. Abst. 20:10-74.

Scoville, W. B., and B. Milner. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20:11-21.

Squire, L. R., and S. Zola-Morgan. (1983). The neurology of memory: The case for correspondence between the findings for human and nonhuman primates. In J. A. Deutsch, Ed., The Physiological Basis of Memory. New York: Academic Press, pp. 199-268.

Suzuki, W. A., and D. G. Amaral. (1994). Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: Cortical afferents. J. Comp. Neurol. 350:497-533.

Suzuki, W. A., S. Zola-Morgan, L. R. Squire, and D. G. Amaral. (1993). Lesions of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices in the monkey produce long-lasting memory impairment in the visual and tactual modalities. J. Neurosci. 13:2430-2451.

Zola-Morgan, S. M., and L. R. Squire. (1985). Amnesia in monkeys following lesions of the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus. Ann. Neurol. 17:558-564.

Zola-Morgan, S. M., and L. R. Squire. (1993). Neuroanatomy of memory. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 16:547-563.

Zola-Morgan, S., L. R. Squire, D. G. Amaral, and W. A. Suzuki. (1989). Lesions of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex that spare the amygdala and hippocampal formation produce severe memory impairment. J. Neurosci. 9:4355-4370.

Further Readings

Alvarado, M. C., and J. W. Rudy. (1995). Rats with damage to the hippocampal-formation are impaired on the transverse- patterning problem but not on elemental discriminations. Behav. Neurosci. 109:204-211.

Alvarez-Royo, P., R. P. Clower, S. Zola-Morgan, and L. R. Squire. (1991). Stereotaxic lesions of the hippocampus in monkeys: Determination of surgical coordinates and analysis of lesions using magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. Methods 38:223-232.

Amaral, D. G., Ed. (1991). Is the hippocampal formation preferentially involved in spatial behavior? Hippocampus (special issue) 1:221-292.

Bunsey, M., and H. Eichenbaum. (1995). Selective damage to the hippocampal region blocks long-term retention of a natural and nonspatial stimulus-stimulus association. Hippocampus 5:546-556.

Eichenbaum, H. (1997). Declarative memory: Insights from cognitive neurobiology. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48:547-572.

Eichenbaum, H., T. Otto, and N. J. Cohen. (1994). Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system. Behav. and Brain Sci. 17:449-517.

Horel, J. A., D. E. Pytko-Joiner, M. Voytko, and K. Salsbury. (1987). The performance of visual tasks while segments of the inferotemporal cortex are suppressed by cold. Behav. Brain Res. 23:29-42.

Jaffard, R., and M. Meunier. (1993). Role of the hippocampal formation in learning and memory. Hippocampus 3:203-218.

Jarrard, L. E. (1993). On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. Behav. Neural Biol. 60:9-26.

Jarrard, L. E., and B. S. Meldrum. (1993). Selective excitotoxic pathology in the rat hippocampus. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 19:381-389.

Mair, R. G., C. D. Anderson, P. J. Langlais, and W. J. McEntree. (1988). Behavioral impairments, brain lesions and monoaminergic activity in the rat following a bout of thiamine deficiency. Behav. Brain Res. 27:223-239.

Mishkin, M. (1978). Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not separate removal of the amygdala and hippocampus. Nature 273:297-298.

Nadel, L. (1995). The role of the hippocampus in declarative memory: A comment on Zola-Morgan, Squire, and Ramus (1994). Hippocampus 5:232-234.

Vnek, N., T. C. Gleason, and L. F. Kromer. (1995). Entorhinal- hippocampal connections and object memory in the rat: Acquisition versus retention. J. Neurosci. 15:3193-3199.

Zola-Morgan, S., L. R. Squire, and S. J. Ramus. (1995). The role of the hippocampus in declarative memory: A reply to Nadel. Hippocampus 5:235-239.